PREDICTING RETALIATION IN THE WORKPLACE THE THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOR AND ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE
diegola3 de Noviembre de 2012
2.718 Palabras (11 Páginas)920 Visitas
Dr. NW
Calgary, AB, Canada, T2N 1N4
INTRODUCTION
Counterproductive behavior in the workplace can be costly for organizations; up to 75% of
employees engage in theft, sabotage, unexcused absenteeism, or vandalism (Harper, 1990). Of
the many predictors of deviant workplace behavior, perceived unfairness is among the strongest
(Bennett & Robinson, 2000). Counterproductive behavior that occurs in response to unfair
treatment often represents an attempt to restore justice by “getting even” with a person or
organization, and is referred to as retaliation (Skarlicki & Folger, 1997).
In this study, I investigated how employees’ perceptions of unfair treatment predict their
decisions to retaliate against their supervisors and organization. Specifically, I tested whether
individuals who perceive different types of injustice direct their retaliation at different targets. To
account for other contextual factors, I also tested whether the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen,
1991) offers a useful theoretical framework for understanding retaliation in the workplace.
The Theory of Planned Behavior
The theory of planned behavior (TPB, Ajzen, 1991) states that behavioral intentions are the
most immediate precursor of behavior and, in turn, intentions are predicted by behavioral
control, expected utility, and perceived norms. As applied to retaliation, behavioral control refers
to beliefs about the existence of safe opportunities to “get back”. Support for behavioral control
as a predictor of retaliation comes from studies showing that employees who perceived a low,
rather than high, chance of punishment were more likely to model coworkers’ anti-social
behavior (Robinson & O’Leary-Kelly, 1998), and employees who perceived a low, rather than
high, possibility of detection were over three times as likely to steal (Hollinger & Clark, 1983).
Expected utility refers to the extent to which an individual believes that the outcomes of
retaliating are worth the potential costs. Retaliation offers a means of restoring self-esteem and
satisfying a desire for revenge (Bies & Tripp, 1996). Retaliation also allows one to maintain a
belief in a just world (Lerner, 1980) by causing something “bad” to happen to a “bad” person
who perpetrated an injustice. Individuals calculate the costs and benefits of retaliation (Bies &
Tripp, 2001) and may be more likely to retaliate if such an action is deemed worthwhile.
Perceived norms refer to beliefs about whether “significant others” might approve of
retaliation. Counterproductive behaviors are predicted by antisocial climates (Robinson &
O’Leary-Kelly, 1998) and employees are absent more often when it is common among their
coworkers (Johns, 1997). Moreover, theft is influenced by employees’ beliefs about how a
thieving coworker would be judged (Shaprio, Trevino, & Victor, 1995).
The above review suggests that the TPB predictors may influence decisions to retaliate. I
hypothesized that behavioral control, expected utility, and perceived norms predict intent and,
ultimately, retaliation against supervisors (H1a, H1b, H1c) and organizations (H2a, H2b, H2c).
Academy of Management Best Conference Paper 2003 OB: L1
Organizational Justice and Retaliation
Researchers have examined the perceived fairness of outcomes (distributive justice),
procedures (procedural justice), and treatment from authorities (interactional justice) (Folger &
Cropanzano, 1998). Interactional justice comprises perceptions of interpersonal justice, which
refers to the extent that authorities treat employees with dignity and respect, and informational
justice, which is the extent to which authorities provide adequate explanations for decisions
(Colquitt, 2001; Greenberg, 1993). Regardless of its form, injustice represents a loss of
something to which individuals feel entitled (Folger & Cropanzano, 1998); hence, people seek
retribution to restore justice (Bies & Tripp, 1996).
Bennett and Robinson (2002) proposed that researchers should examine the relative impact
of different types of justice on various forms of deviance. The agent-system model (Masterson,
Lewis, Goldman, & Taylor, 2000) suggests that the source of procedural justice is typically the
organization, whereas the source of interactional justice is often a supervisor. Thus, given social
exchange principles, procedural justice should predict retaliation against organizations (RAO),
and interactional justice should be related to retaliation against supervisors (RAS).
In addition to RAS, interactional justice might also predict RAO. Individuals who are treated
unfairly by a supervisor might retaliate against the organization that their supervisor represents
(Ambrose, Seabright, & Schminke, 2002), which might also prevent a cycle of “tit for tat” with
an unfair supervisor (Aquino, Lewis, & Bradfield, 1999). Indeed, studies have shown that
interactional justice predicts retaliation toward both the organization and its members (i.e.,
coworkers and supervisors), but procedural justice tends to predict only retaliation against the
organization (Ambrose et al., 2002; Aquino et al., 1999; Henle, 2002). In general, interactional
justice might be a stronger predictor of retaliation because it is easier to attribute blame for
mistreatment by a supervisor than for an unfair procedure (Folger & Cropanzano, 1998), and
blame is a critical component of revenge (e.g., Bies & Tripp, 1996).
In this study, interactional justice was assessed through informational and interpersonal
justice (Colquitt, 2001). I hypothesized that informational and interpersonal justice would predict
RAS (H3a, H3b) and RAO (H4a, H4b). Procedural justice was expected to predict RAO (H5),
and informational and interpersonal justice were hypothesized to be stronger predictors of RAS
(H6a, H6b) and RAO (H7a, H7b) than procedural justice. Because justice-retaliation
relationships are well-supported, all hypothesized effects were tested stringently by examining
them incremental to the other justice constructs and the TPB variables.
METHODOLOGY
Participants, Procedure, and Measures
Participants (N = 96) were employed individuals taking courses at a Canadian university
(67% female, age: M = 22.4, SD = 3.99). On average, participants had held over four jobs,
resulting in 6.5 years of work experience. Participants had been employed in their present
organization for over two years in various industries including service (37.5%), sales (23%),
professional (13.5%), and others (26%). I sampled from multiple organizations because it might
result in: (a) greater variance in, for example, the perceived risk of being detected retaliating, (b)
increased generalizability (Bennett & Robinson, 2002), and (c) reduced response distortion that
might have resulted from concerns about admitting to retaliation in an organization-wide survey.
Academy of Management Best Conference Paper 2003 OB: L2
Participants completed questionnaires in a quiet room and were not asked to name
themselves or their employer. The order of whether the justice measures were completed before
(n = 49) or after (n = 47) the retaliation and TPB measures was counterbalanced.
Organizational justice variables were assessed using Colquitt’s (2001) items. Response
options ranged from 1 (to a small extent) to 7 (to a large extent) for interpersonal (four items,
Cronbach’s alpha = .92), informational (five items, alpha = .90), procedural (seven items, alpha
= .76), and distributive justice (four items, alpha = .96).
Each TPB item was assessed on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). TPB
items for RAS and RAO included “my immediate supervisor” or “the organization for which I
work”, respectively, and consisted of intentions (I intend to get back at or settle the score
with…), behavioral control (If I was mistreated by…, I could easily do things to settle the score
with little chance that I would be caught), expected utility (If I was mistreated by…, the
satisfaction of getting even would outweigh the risks of getting caught), and perceived norms (If
my coworkers were mistreated by…, many of them would try to get back in some way).
Items used to assess retaliation were very similar to items used in previous research (Aquino
et al., 1999; Robinson & O’Leary-Kelly, 1998; Skarlicki & Folger, 1997), and were assessed
through self-report because retaliation is often enacted in private (Bennett & Robinson, 2000).
Seven items assessed RAO (alpha = .72), five of which were taken from Bennett and Robinson
(2000), and the response format ranged from 1 (never) to 7 (daily): “Taken property from work
without permission”, “Taken an additional or longer break than is acceptable at your workplace”,
“Put little effort into your work”, “Intentionally worked slower than you could have worked”,
and “Spent too much time fantasizing or daydreaming instead of working”. Two items
constructed for this study were assessed
...